
Such events are exactly those most frequently studied in unrealistic optimism research (Welkenhuysen, Evers-Kieboom, Decruyenaere, & van den Berghe (p. As we show next, problems stem from the fact that for very rare events the sizeable majority of people will be less at risk than the average person. The most popular scale used in the comparative method is -3 to +3 (e.g., ). We then go on to conduct three empirical tests to determine what evidence for comparative optimism is observed when controlling for these statistical confounds. The detail underlying these mechanisms is provided in, but here we provide a brief description of these mechanisms. Moreover, for non-omniscient, but non-optimistic rational agents, base rate regression was another statistical mechanism leading to seemingly biased responses. Harris and Hahn demonstrated how seemingly optimistic results could be obtained even from agents who had perfect knowledge about their future, through the mechanisms of scale attenuation and minority undersampling. This is taken as evidence of optimism, since we desire not to experience negative events.Īlthough the logic underlying the test is sound, in practice its data are compromised by statistical artifacts. The typical result is that, for negative events, the average score is less than zero. Therefore, if the average response on this scale differs from zero, this is taken as evidence for a systematic underlying bias at the group level. The logic of the test is that, although each participant’s own risk can be greater or less than the average person’s, the average of all participants’ risks should, by definition, be the average risk. Participants report their answer by circling a number between -3 (much less likely than the average person) and +3 (much more likely than the average person).

The unequivocality of evidence in favor of unrealistic optimism has, however, recently been questioned. It is against this background that Nobel Laureate Daniel Kahneman described optimism as “the most significant of the cognitive biases” (p. Applied practitioners within health psychology, for example, have been concerned that if individuals perceive risks as more relevant to the average person than to themselves, individuals will not take appropriate protective behavior against major risks. Since Weinstein’s seminal paper, a huge number of academic papers (e.g., ) and popular psychological books (e.g., ) have expressed this view, ensuring its prevalence not only in social psychology, but also amongst applied practitioners and laypeople (e.g., ). These statements represent the dominant position in the literature.

People tend to think they are invulnerable and that bad things will happen to others, not to them.
